perm filename LEC[AM,DBL] blob sn#500066 filedate 1980-04-17 generic text, type C, neo UTF8
COMMENT āŠ—   VALID 00003 PAGES
C REC  PAGE   DESCRIPTION
C00001 00001
C00002 00002	Here is the text of an informal talk I gave to Simon's UNDERSTAND
C00004 00003	I started preparing for this talk by asking my colleagues in Computer
C00064 ENDMK
CāŠ—;
Here is the text of an informal talk I gave to Simon's UNDERSTAND
seminar last  yeat, and to my CS301 class (The CS Profession) this year.
I borrowed liberally from Winston's
notes and from Strunk & White.

> If the theme of "origins of lecturing heuristics" is worth preserving
   Then we must follow through (toward the end I degenerated into merely
   giving country advice, without analyzing its source of power).

> In any event, much more material should probably be incorporated
    >> Perhaps as an "open" project (a bulletin board of sorts)
       Where a large community acesses, adds to,... a growing body of rules
         >>> Maybe even tied into what Ira Goldstein  is doing vis a vis the
             Arpanet (Semantic net of SIGART readers' profiles)

Do you have any suggestions?  


Doug
I started preparing for this talk by asking my colleagues in Computer
Science what  they considered to be the  most important heuristic for
lecturing.


[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[

Make sure you're wearing clothes.
                                                           -- Professor 1


Make sure your fly is zipped.
                                                            -- Professor 2
]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]


While there was surprising  agreement in their answers, this probably
reflects more on my choice of friends than on effective lecturing.

INTERRUPT!  When I stand over on this side <<META>>, I'll be making a
comment about this talk itself, a meta-level remark.

I've just  opened my talk in a very  weak, ineffectual way.  What was
wrong with the way I did it?  There are really 3 mistakes:


[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[


                            THREE   MISTAKES




>  OPENING WITH A JOKE


>  GIVING NO CARROT


>  GIVING NO OUTLINE

]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]


Opening  with a  joke is  usually a  disaster.  In  the first fifteen
minutes, people  are  looking for  pencils, they're  still  arriving,
they're not responsive yet.   They don't understand what you're doing
in  the lecture, and by  the time they realize  that what you started
with was supposed to be funny, most of its impact is dissipated.

Second, and more importantly, I haven't given you a statement of what
will  be learned.  It's  important to present such  a "carrot" early,
and to be excited by it.

Third, you  still have  no idea  what this  talk is  going to  cover.
Somehow, I need to communicate to you a concise overview of what this
talk is all  about. The best way I've found to  do that is to present
an  OUTLINE of  the talk,  and to keep  redisplaying it  now and then
during the hour, checking off each point as it's covered.


[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[



                                OUTLINE



A WEAK OPENING

A CARROT

HEURISTICS BASED UPON I.P.S. LIMITS

MORE SOPHISTICATED HEURISTICS
        Based upon Acculturation
        Based upon Individual Personality
        Based upon Psychosociology

MORE PRIMITIVE HEURISTICS
        Based upon Physics
        Based upon Physiology

]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]

<<check off "Weak Opening">>

Let's go back down into the  talk itself, and rectify mistakes 2 & 3.
<<SHIFT out of META level>> Let me start again.

Perhaps the most common  opinion about effective speaking and writing
is  that they depend upon  innate gifts, that they  cannot be taught.
We in  AI and Information Processing Psychology are  in a position to
attempt  to de-mystify  some of this.   While it's  very difficult to
work out operational rules for good lecturing, we hypothesize that it
can be done.  This talk will present many such "heuristics", rules of
thumb.   We'll see how  some of  them fall right  out from well-known
paramenters of the human  information processing system.  Some of the
heuristics have even more  primitive roots, from humans as biological
organisms,  or even  from humans  viewed merely  as physical objects.
Finally, some heuristics have a much more sophisticated grounding, in
theories of personality, both individual and societal.

Not  only should you come  away with a sense  of how these heuristics
work  -- how  they de-mystify effective  lecturing --  but I'm hoping
that at least a few of  the individual heuristics will be new to you,
that you'll  be able  to use  them in  your talks.   <<Check off  the
"carrot" on the outline>>

<<META  LEVEL:>> Notice that  this organization is far  from the only
one:  on first blush it  might seem that the most natural thing would
have been  to group  all  the heuristics  together which  dealt  with
choosing a  topic, all the rules  for engineering sentences together,
and  so on, regardless  of their origins.  That  would certainly have
been  better  if  my   only  goal  were  to  convey  some  individual
techniques.  I chose this outline to make it easier for me to satisfy
my other  goal: to  show  you the  straightforward origins  of  these
techniques.

<<Back from META>> For each  of these levels, then, we'll review some
facts,  some more or  less well-accepted results, and  based on those
facts we'll propose some heuristics for lecturing.


*********************  IPS level  **********************************

First, consider the level which  is probably most familiar to most of
you in  this  room: the  level  at  which a  human  is viewed  as  an
information  processing  system,  a  symbol  manipulator,  a  problem
solver.

We'll assume that the human can be modelled by a set of memories...






<< IPS diagram from Newell and Simon's HPS>>






...(holding facts  and methods)  and internal  processors,  organized
just as  Newell and Simon describe in Human  Problem Solving.  As for
the  detailed characteristics  of these memories  and channels, we'll
accept   some  of   the  more  fundamental   results  from  cognitive
psychology:


[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[

                       SOME  FAMILIAR  PROPERTIES
                         OF  THE  HUMAN  I.P.S.

    STM
        Small  (7 +- 2)
        Retain 2 while another task is run
        Amodal
        Writing: 50 milliseconds/symbol
        Reading: 50 milliseconds/symbol
    LTM
                     10
        Infinite  (10   >> 256k)
        Associative
        Amodal
        Writing: 10 seconds/chunk
        Reading: 300 milliseconds/symbol
    EM  (VISUAL)
        Large
        Writing: 1 second/symbol
        Reading: 100 milliseconds/symbol
    PROCESSOR
        Serial
        EIP time: 50 milliseconds
        Production system
        Goal-directed

]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]



A typical  spoken sentence  will span  about 4  seconds, and  contain
10-15   words.   There's  not   enough  time  to   be  guaranteed  of
transferring even a single symbol into LTM; 2 of the locations in STM
will  probably retain some  information from the  last sentence after
this one  is ended, meaning that only 5  symbols can be stored there.
If  we don't speak the  language of the speaker,  then each symbol is
probably the size of a single syllable.  If we speak his language but
can't make any sense out the sentence, then each symbol is probably a
word.  So in  that case, we could absorb and  mull over a sentence of
five  words or less.   If we're  very familiar with  the language and
with the topic being discussed,  then of course the words are chunked
together,  and a much  more complex sentence can  be understood.  But
note  that in no case  can we be  expected to keep more  than about 5
items  on our "stack".  If  we have too many  pieces presented before
they're tied together, some of them will have slipped away.

Look at this situation from  the other side, now.  Suppose you have a
thought you want to communicate.   In order to maximize the number of
listeners  who'll  understand  you, speak  in  short  sentences,  and
eliminate needless words from each sentence.  These are two tenets of
an excellent book: Strunk & White's ELEMENTS of STYLE.

"Because the last  few words of a sentence will  be most likely to be
retained (at least for  several seconds), it's important to place the
emphatic words near the sentence's end."

That sentence would have been less effective had I reversed the order
of the clauses ...

[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[


               Because the last  few words of  a sentence
               will  be most  likely to  be  retained (at
               least for several seconds), it's important
               to   place   emphatic   words   near   the
               sentence's end.


               Place emphatic  words near  the sentence's
               end,  because  the  last  few  words  of a
               sentence  will   be  most  likely   to  be
               retained (at least for several seconds).

]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]


...for then  you'd think I was saying  something about word retention
as a  function of  position;  wheras I  was actually  recommending  a
certain  placement policy.   This idea  has a  global analogue: close
your  talk with a dramatic  sentence: end with a  joke or a memorable
phrase or both (e.g., the heuristic use of heuristic beings).

This whole  problem of limited STM size is  relieved by providing the
audience  with an appropriate EM  upon which to gaze:  say a vu-graph
containing the offending sentence.  This is especially useful for the
case where you genuinely do  have many premises to state, followed by
a conclusion that finally  ties them together into a meaningful unit.

The solution  is easily understood by  looking at the characteristics
of  foveal vision.   The  same  justification  can be  made  for  the
following  heuristic:   use   vivid,  concrete  imagery.  Prefer  the
specific to  the general, the definite to the  vague, the concrete to
the abstract.  E.g.:



[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[


               He   showed   satisfaction   as   he  took
               possession of his well-earned reward.


               He grinned as he pocketed the coin.

]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]


The surest way to arouse and hold the attention of the listener is by
being specific, definite, and  concrete.  The great writers -- Homer,
Dante, Shakespeare  -- are  effective largely  because they  deal  in
particulars,  they report details  that matter.  Their  words call up
pictures in our minds.  Those  pictures are almost as accessable as a
vu-graph image, as a "genuine" EM.

I  believe   that  this  same  virtual   EM  mechanism  explains  the
effectiveness of  examples.  A good example,  which the speaker keeps
referring back to as he  develops his argument, is a concrete anchor,
a  specific  image which  contains  easily-accessed  rich  structure.
Failure to  have such an archetype can at  times be quite frustrating
to the listener, as e.g., right now.

Returning to  our list  -- actually  Newell and  Simon's list  --  of
invariant characteristics of the human information processing system,
we examine LTM.  The most macroscopic property is the slow write time
(10 seconds/chunk).   If a  complex idea  is composed  of ten  lesser
ones,  each of which  is comprised  of ten primitive  chunks, then we
can't  expect to learn more  than 3 or  4 such new ideas  in a 1-hour
lecture.  This has two important corollaries:

Cor. 1:   Humans can learn an idea in  an hour lecture.  Hence, don't
hesitate to insert an idea.

Cor. 2:  Humans cannot  assimilate 40 ideas/hour.  If you really have
that  many ideas worth  communicating, you should  re-think the whole
situation.  Perhaps they should come out in book form, or distributed
over several  articles.   More likely,  you'll  find a  couple  major
themes emerging, plus an array of small related items.

<<META:  >>  E.g.,  in  this  lecture,  we  have  the  theme  of  the
explicability  of effective  lecturing, plus we  have many individual
heuristics that are worth  knowing.  In such a situation, the typical
solution is to take written  notes, to transfer the ideas from STM to
EM.  Another common solution  is REPETITION. A lecturer may place his
notes and vugraphs in the  library, in order that students may review
it  at their own  pace.  A  more comprehensive kind  of repetition is
frequently  the solution imposed  on students who  don't take written
notes  in class  and  don't  review the  material  afterwards.   More
seriously,   the  idea  of  teaching   through  repetition  is  quite
efficacious.  Cycle  in  on difficult  ideas,  both within  a  single
lecture and in a complete course.

Note-taking is not satisfactory.  Consider the tremendous cost of the
STM-->EM transfer.   Probably the listener can't  attend fully to the
speaker when he's furiously taking notes, certainly he can't spend as
much processing  power assimilating the material.   After all, he's a
uniprocessor.   This dilemma explains  why it's so  effective to pass
out  a detailed outline  of the talk  you're giving, so  that all the
"content"  is  there  already.   Listeners  need  jot  down  only  an
occasional remark or two.

<<PASS OUT OUTLINE>>

Even worse  than trying to cover 40 separate  points in a single hour
is  the attempt  to convey  40 ideas in  order, where  you must fully
grasp the earlier ones in  order to be ABLE to understand the current
one.   A  common  case   of  this  vicious  dependency  is  found  in
mathematics  colloquia,  where  what   is  really  a  large  tree  of
propositions  is embedded  in a linear  order and  presented that way
(lemma, proof, lemma, proof,..., theorem, proof).

The  problem with that  is simply that not  much is going  to make it
into LTM;  very little  will  be retrievable  30 minutes  later.   My
objection evaporates,  of  course, if  one  is presenting  a  written
article.   Analogously,  during   a  talk  one  can  write  important
definitions  and  lemmata  on the  board,  or  distribute  a  handout
containing them.

The human  IPS processor  is  able to  work just  a bit  faster  than
necessary  to keep up with  the 3 words/second rate  of human speech.
What is  happening during the rest of  those cycles?  Assimilation of
the new material, no doubt: tying it in to already-known concepts.  I
hypothesize that  if the listener doesn't  understand what's said, he
uses this computational power to try to make sense out of it (to find
his  own examples of  it, to "punctuate" it,  etc.).  This processing
can be  anticipated  and facilitated  by  the speaker,  by  preparing
audio-visual aids  which  exemplify, clarify,  and pucutate  what  is
being said.   It may aid STM to duplicate  exactly what's being said,
but  it won't aid  in understanding.  For that,  you must SUPPLEMENT,
not merely replicate, what's being said...

[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[
     Colorful slides supplement furiously
]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]

...as that one does.

Another reason for slides is that often verbal description is not the
right "language" to use to  convey some idea.  A graph or picture may
be appropriate,  or  a program  listing.  Don't  underestimate  human
ability for  empirical induction:  often, if  you can't describe some
idea  well, you can  still convey  it by a  few well-chosen examples,
non-examples, and near-misses.

Another way to rationalize the use of supplemental slides is that the
human  IPS is  goal-directed:  it  tries to  understand.   The  aural
signals  emanating from the  podium are a major  source of knowledge,
but so can be the visual images being projected.  The synergy between
cooperating knowledge sources is much in the news these days.



If we  stop focussing just on low-level,  local, short-range goals of
the human,  then we  leave the  domain of  applicability of  data  on
characteristics of the IPS.


[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[


               If  we stop  focussing just  on low-level,
               local,  short-range  goals  of  the human,
               then we leave the domain  of applicability
               of data on characteristics of the IPS.

               If  we  look  at  some  higher-level, more
               abstract, global, long-range goals  of the
               human,  then  we  enter  the  realm  where
               theories  of  personality   and  sociology
               apply.

]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]


<<META: >>  Notice how  much more  effective it  would have  been  to
phrase that sentence in  the positive, to indicate where we're going,
rather  than where we no  longer are.  An even  more effective way of
conveying the idea  of this shift is to put  back the outline of this
talk and point at it:

<<Back from META>>
<<SLIDE: Display the OUTLINE, again>>

If we worry about all the  goals of the human, then we jump from this
topic down  to this one <<On the OUTLINE  slide, point from IPS LEVEL
to  More Sophisticated  LEVEL>>.  After we  do that,  we'll regress a
bit, and discuss some of the more nitty-gritty heuristics.



***********************  Sophisticated heurs  *********************

Some of  these heuristics  stem from  our training,  our  educational
experiences, our culture.   For instance, we've become conditioned to
a one-hour  attention span  of  sorts, and  it's dangerous  to  speak
longer than that.

One  important  class  of heuristics  deal  with  using  language  to
advantage.

[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[



                             EFFECTIVE  USE
                              OF  LANGUAGE


RULES OF USAGE
    A participial phrase at the beginning of a
    sentence must refer to the grammatical subject

RULES OF COMPOSITION
    Choose a suitable design and hold to it
    Use the active voice
    Put statements in positive form
    Place negative and positive in opposition
  * Use definite, specific, concrete language
  * Omit needless words
    Express co-ordinate ideas in parallel form
    Keep related words together
    Keep to one tense
  * Place the emphatic words at the end

RULES OF FORM
    Worry about colloquialisms, hyphens, exclamations

]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]


We've  already discussed  "omit needless words",  and "place emphatic
words  at  the  ends of  sentences",  and  "use  definite,  specific,
concrete language".  There are many more:

Some are rules of USAGE  ("a participial phrase at the beginning of a
sentence must refer to the grammatical subject")

[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[
          Being  in a dilapidated condition,  I was able to
          buy the house very cheap.
]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]

Some are  rules of  COMPOSITION: "The  active voice  is usually  more
direct and vigorous than  the passive". Similarly, "put statements in
positive form." Placing negative and positive in opposition makes for
a stronger structure.

[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[
Not charity, but simple justice.
Not that I loved Caesar less, but that I loved Rome more.
]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]

Express co-ordinate ideas in similar form, i.e., use parallel construction.

[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[
A heuristic is not a rule of inference, merely a rule of thumb.
]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]

Keep related words together...

[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[
    New York's  first commercial human-sperm  bank opened Friday
    with semen samples from 18 men frozen in a stainless steel tank.
]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]

...  In  this version of the sentence, the  listener's heart would go
out to those 18 poor fellows frozen in a steel tank.

Some high-level  rules of  FORM exist:  take care  how your  audience
might   misinterpret  various  sentences,   ideas,  expressions.   In
Chattanooga, when  2 newspapers merged, the NEWS  and the FREE PRESS,
they  blindly hyphenated  them  to  get  the new  paper's  name:  the
Chatanooga News-Free Press.

This  set of rules  is of  course more exemplary  than exhaustive; as
Strunk and  White put  it, who  can say  confidently what  ignites  a
certain  combination of words,  causing them to explode  in the mind?
Every speaker,  by  the  way in  which  he wields  language,  reveals
something  of his  spirit, his  habits, his  capacities, his biasses.
This is both inevitable and enjoyable.  But don't confuse spontaneity
with  genius.  A breezy  style, well  maybe an iota  more breezy than
mine, say, is the mark of an egocentric.  In general, if you're going
to  deviate from the  cultural norms, from accepted  rules of grammar
and style,  do  it  for a  reason  in  each case:  be  intentionally,
purposivley  provacative or ingenious.  Perhaps  the highest ideal at
this  level is  clarity.   When  you  become hopelessly  mired  in  a
sentence, stop  and start  afresh: don't  try to  hack your  way  out
against the terrible odds of syntax; usually what's wrong is that the
sentence  construction has become  too involved at  some point. Start
over, using much shorter, simpler sentence constructions.

When you say something, make sure that you have said it.  The chances
of your having said it will then be at least fair.

Avoid jargon as much as  possible, even in a technical talk.  We tend
to drag  in a pouch of  private words whose only  virtue is that they
are  exceptionally nimble and  can escape from the  garden of meaning
over the wall.  Learn to spot words that at first seem frieghted with
meaning, but then soon burst  in air, leaving nothing but a memory of
bright   sound.     Even   worse    than   jargon    buzzwords    are
PROGRAM-VARIABLE-NAMES, using  the actual  program's print-names,  no
matter whether they're the best possible names or not.  Unless you're
giving  a minicourse on how  to run and modify  a particular piece of
software, it can only help  to rename variables so as to be maximally
suggestive.


Perhaps the  ultimate  customizing method  at  this "word"  level  is
sentence engineering:   rework a sentence until  it will have meaning
at  several levels, so  that everyone in your  expected audience will
think they understand it.

[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[

A heuristic is really just compiled hindsight.

A.M. creates new concepts and explores facets of them.

]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]


Note that the first  sentence can be understood by someone unfamiliar
with the technical computer science meaning of compilation, though of
course  one gets  more out of  the sentence if  he has  and uses that
meaning.  In  the second sentence, one can  still extract some useful
meaning,  even he doesn't  know that  concepts and facets  refer to a
particular, well-defined data structure in the A.M. system.

Always  think  of  your audience,  both  locally  when  you  engineer
sentences, and globally when you plan your talk.

It is important to realize  that a lecture for CS104, introduction to
programming,  is quite  different from  the Turing  award speech.  By
isolating the  overall goals  of  the speaker  and audience,  we  can
categorize a few situations worth distinguishing.


[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[


    Technical
         Peer Audience
         Job Interview
         Invited Talk
    Casual
         How I Spent my Summer Vacation

    Popular
         AI for Tots
         After-dinner Speech


]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]


In a continuing course, the  instructor must bear in mind his general
course  goals, and each  lecture must fit into  this high-level plan.
In  a  technical  seminar,  it  would  be  disastrous  to  under-  or
over-estimate the  level of  expertise of  the audience.   At a  talk
you're giving  to get a job interview, the  goals are obviously quite
different than  if  you've been  asked to  make  a few  remarks  upon
accepting some award.  When you  compose and edit your talk, you must
filter  it  past  the   goals:  does  it  satisfy  your  goals?   the
audience's?

MY purpose...

[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[


TO MOTIVATE
    Show Results
    Entertain
          jokes
          question the audience
          intentionally trigger questions
          home demonstration
          visual effects, props, gimmicks
          multi-media, multi-screen
          gestures
          eccentricity
          eye contact
          approach audience
          approach screen
          professional graphics
          emergency time-sink
          crowded rooms
          don't get deflected

]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]

... in delivering talks -- at least, talks about my research -- is to
both   convey  knowledge,   and  to  get   the  audience  interested.
Recognizing  that little knowledge can  be permanently transferred in
an  hour,   I  concentrate  on  motivating.   If  the  listener  gets
interested,  he or she  will talk to me  in more detail,  go and read
some articles on the project, and thus -- in TENS of hours -- acquire
a grasp of what I'm working on.

How  do you motivate  an audience?  Part of  it is to  show them some
interesting results,  to arouse their curiosity  in HOW those results
were  acheived, to  convince them  that there  is some  power here if
they're  willing  to  study   it.   Another  part  of  motivating  is
entertainment, both for its own  sake and to assure the listener that
should he choose to pursue this, he'll find further material which is
comprehensible and enjoyable.

Be  careful about jokes and  puns: if they fail  it's a real disaster
(unless you  are skilled at appealing to  the audience for sympathy).
But a  few good  jokes  can brighten  a talk  tremendously.   Knuth's
criterion  for a good joke  is that it be  TECHNICAL, that it require
some detailed understanding of  the current material, in order to get
the joke...

[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[

                            TECHNICAL  JOKES


On maps, thick red lines constrain furiously.

The heuristic use of heuristic beings.

The "Rock Test" parody of Turing's test.

]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]

...I  can think of  a couple reasons for  this:  (1) When  we get the
joke,  we reward  our  own  cleverness  --  and publicize  it  --  by
laughing.  (2) As Arthur  Koestler says, the discovery of the jarring
of two separate matrices  of thought is what creates the appreciation
of  humor, and in this  case we have two  simultaneous such jarrings,
one for the joke itself,  and one because the joke is highly relevant
to the current discussion.

A  technique which,  like jokes,  can work  well or  can backfire, is
asking  questions  of  the audience.   It  helps  convert  a  passive
situation into an active participation.

An even more subtle technique  is to arrange your discourse, and even
your slides, in such a way that a particular question is just begging
to  be asked.   If that  question is  on several  people's minds, and
especially if someone voices  it, then it will be extremely effective
when you answer it, and in fact when that answer flows into the talk.

An excellent way to motivate is to propose some little self-contained
experiment or  demonstration they  can try  later, on  their own,  to
confirm a point of the lecture.

Most talks are delivered in  such a routine way that any variation at
all  is likely  to sustain  interest. Use  visual effects, especially
props and complete demonstrations.

Get  a few emergency  tricks, in  case you feel  an audience slipping
away. Try dropping a rock, or asking people to keep their eye on this
point <<at this moment, place a  paper cut-out of a Xerox image of my
hand (pointing)  on  the vu-graph,  creating  the illusion  that  I'm
pointing; then walk away and continue talking non-chalently>>.

Try  appealing  to  several  senses  (almost  any  recording  in  the
background  is  a  novelty),  try composing  some  of  the  above  or
compounding  them (3  slide projectors); (writing  [esp: with colored
chalk]  on vu-graph  image projected  onto the  blackboard instead of
onto the screen).

Cultivate  gestures.  Point  at the board  a lot.   This is effective
even if what you point at is unrelated to what you're saying.

Have  an eccentricity.   Chew tobacco,  wear three  piece suits <<I'm
currently  in a  three piece suit  as I say  this>>, or  point at the
board a lot.

Look people in the eye  often.  This establishes that you are talking
with them,  rather than  presenting  material as  if to  a  videotape
machine.

Try to break down the natural barrier that exists between speaker and
listener.   Walk toward the audience  and away from them,  as well as
left to right across the room.  <<Act this out, of course>>

Get physically close to any projected images.  <<Act this out>> Aside
from the  entertainment aspect, this has a  god IPS justification: it
doesn't  force people to  divide their attention between  you and the
screen.

Similarly,  don't distract  people  by  showing them  a  half-covered
slide.  They'll divide their  attention, spending some time trying to
guess  what's underneath.  <<Demonstrate this>> Instead,  I find it's
much better to just use overlays: that way, the audience doesn't know
that anything's coming until it's there already.<<Overlay: "See!">>


Neat drawings and slides,  particularly in color, create the illusion
that  the lecturer is at  least twice as well  organized as he really
is.   At IJCAI, several of  my slides were not  quite legible, except
for their headings.  Yet, because they  -- and the rest of my talk --
seemed so well organized, many people came up to me and congratulated
me  for calculating so effectively  just what material on  each of my
slides  was  supposed  to be  legible,  and  what  was  intentionally
illegible.

On  the same point,  carry a pipe  or a half-empty  coffee cup.  That
way, if  you get  stuck, stop  and think  while puffing  or  sipping.
<<Have the coffee cup ready for this point, of course>>

Many of the entertainment  heuristics had to do with mass psychology.
There are some more along those lines that we should all know:

Avoid sparsely populated lecture halls.  Empty seats suggest, whether
true or not, that attendance is much lower than expected.  Just a few
people forced to stand, <<as in this case -- hopefully>> on the other
hand, suggests the opposite. Steeply sloping halls are also bad, like
ScH  7500.  They  give the  effect of standing  at the  bottom of the
grand canyon, lecturing to the stones.

A group  is emotive in proportion to its  size.  A huge audience will
have extreme  positive and negative reactions.   There is a noticable
difference even between 50 and 150 people.

If  you  get  a deflectionary  question,  deflect  it.   Don't  allow
obstructionist questioners to drag you off your plan.  Talk with them
after the lecture, if they wish.


*********************** Primitive Heuristics ********************


<<Display the slide with the  OUTLINE AGAIN>> We're now going to move
on to some heuristics which are based on very primitive views of man.
First, consider Man viewed as a physical entity...

[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[



                           OTHER VIEWS OF MAN


AS A PHYSICAL ENTITY
    Mass
    Opacity
    Unidirectional vocalization
    Unidirectional motion
   ....


AS A BIOLOGICAL ENTITY
    Optical resolving power
    Aural receptive strength
    Need for food
    Limited rate for talking
    Need for sleep
    ESP absent (or undeveloped)

]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]


An important corolloary to  the second point listed (opacity) is that
Man is  not transparent to vu-graph light.  If  you stand in front of
the lens,  the  audience  will no  longer  see  the image.   I  never
understood how  speakers could do such a thing;  I have a very simple
heuristic: I know I'm blocking  the projector iff I notice a blinding
white light  in my eyes <<Demonstrate>>.  I  just found out yesterday
what the  problem was, when Bentley told  me HIS analogous heuristic:
he knows  he's been blocking the projector  for five straight minutes
iff his chest  begins to feel warm.  So this turned  out to be just a
5'6" heuristic after all.

Seriously, we all forget at  times that what the audience sees is the
screen,  NOT what  we  see  illuminated  when  we look  down  on  the
vu-graph.  A  specific danger is that the scales  of graphs will just
barely   miss   being  projected.   <<Demonstrate   this>>   As   for
unidirectionality  of  projected   speech,  well,  make  the  obvious
correction when you turn away from the audience.

If we  view man as a  biological organism, then we  must cater to his
physiological limitations, just as before we viewed him as an IPS and
catered to his information  processing limitations.  In this case, we
must worry  about the resolving power of his  eyes <<tiny font>>, the
<<quietly>> sensitivity  of  his ears,  and  so forth.   Our  limited
speaking rate  has imposed upon us a READING  rate limit (Evelyn Wood
would say,  artificially imposed).  <<too much  print to  read>>,  so
don't  put  too  much  on  any  one  slide.   Humans  need  to  sleep
periodically.  Not only does this  put an upper limit on how long any
speaker  could possibly  take (about 1  week straight),  it gives the
speaker some danger signs  to watch for (napping, yawning).  Finally,
since humans  have low ESP abilities (at  least, not developed much),
it  is generally a  good practice  to define your  terms before using
them.  Also, due to low  ESP abilities, people won't know what you're
omitting from  a talk.  If you run overtime,  be merciless in cutting
out material.  Get right to your conclusions.  I'm  sick and tired of
having a  thoughtless lecturer run 40 minutes  overtime, when most of
his audience is still there only out of courtesy.


<<META: >>  In this  last  section, I've  switched my  attitude  from
friendly  to antagonistic.   Notice, by introspection,  now, how it's
alienated you a  bit, if you picked up on  that tone of hostility and
condescension.  This illustrates a very important heuristic, valuable
enough  to risk having you  leave here angry at  me:  never make your
audience angry  at you; go out  of your way to  show your good faith.
Even  if you know a  lot about the subject  you're speaking on, never
forget that they know more about a host of other topics.  So don't be
condescending; treat your  audience with respect.  Adopt the attitude
that you  will do ANYTHING to help them  understand, that you are not
doing  them a favor,  that they  are worth your  time.  That attitude
will project, and if you stumble the audience will support you.


Just a few words about  closing.  One philosophy says "tell them what
you're going to tell them,  tell them, tell them what you told them".
One unoffensive  way to do the latter is  to put the original outline
back  up, so they can  see what's been covered.   <<Do this>> Another
technique I use  is to put up a new  slide containing the main points
or  themes, or  containing the "open  problems" left  to worry about.
This  is a  good slide  to leave up  during the  ensuing question and
answer period.

[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[[



                         GOALS  OF  THIS  TALK


TO CONVEY INFORMATION
    Heuristics exist for effective lecturing
    Their origins are straightforward

TO MOTIVATE
    Study and master the heuristics
    Investigate rule-guided communication



                              BIBLIOGRAPHY  (v. small font)

Bentley, Jon.  personal communication
Feigenbaum, Edward.  personal communication
Shamos, Michael.  personal communication
Strunk, W., and E. B. White, The Elements of Style, MacMillan, 1972.
Winston, Patrick.  personal communication

]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]]


As to final sentences:  it often is appropriate to close with a joke,
or with a memorable phrase ("the science of science").  The important
thing is to know when  to close. It's horrifying to listen to someone
waste five minutes out of  some compulsion to fill a time slot.  Stop
when finished <<stop abruptly>>.